How secure is the secure base? Romantic attachment, emotion regulation, and psychotherapists’ role as secure base figures

Helena Carvalho,1,4 Gil Nata,2,3 Paula Mena Matos1,41Center for Psychology, University of Porto; 2University of Trás-os-Montes and Alto Douro, Vila Real; 3Center for Research and Intervention in Education, University of Porto; 4Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences, University of Porto, Portugal. Research in Psychotherapy: Psychopathology, Process and Outcome 2024; 27:733 doi:10.4081/ripppo.2024.733

2024 Mar 20

ABSTRACT

In 1988, Bowlby posited that the emotional availability of psychotherapists in establishing a secure base environment is in-fluenced by their personal relational history. Despite the ac-knowledged influence of the therapist’s attachment on therapeutic processes and outcomes, the therapist’s role as a se-cure base figure has received insufficient attention. This study delves into the connection between psychotherapists’ attachment organization and their self-perceived roles as secure base figures within clinical contexts. Additionally, we explore the mediating role of emotion regulation processes in this context. The dataset comprises self-reports from 384 psychotherapists with diverse theoretical orientations. Our analysis reveals both direct and in-direct effects of psychotherapists’ attachment on the provision of a secure base. Emotion regulation, specifically through the dimension of clarity, emerges as a significant mediator in this relationship. This study offers a distinctive contribution to deep-ening our understanding of the relational dynamics inherent to psychotherapeutic practice. It sheds light on the nuanced relation between attachment and emotion regulation, influencing the psy-chotherapist’s role as a secure base figure in psychotherapy. The discussion of the results additionally emphasizes key implica-tions for clinical practice and therapists’ training.

Key words: attachment, secure base, psychotherapist, emotional regulation.

Introduction

Contrasting with Bowlby’s (1988) disappointment concerning the scarce use of the attachment framework in clinical settings, attachment is nowadays considered an important dimension of the psychotherapeutic process and outcome, with the potential to ei-ther facilitate or inhibit psychotherapy effectiveness (Holmes, 2011; Slade & Holmes, 2019). Although with less enthusiasm when compared with the client’s attachment literature, research has been addressing therapists’ attachment as a significant predic-tor of the therapeutic encounter (Daniel, 2006; Degnan et al., 2016; Heinonen & Nissen-Lie, 2020; Lingiardi et al., 2018; Ryan et al., 2023; Steel et al., 2018, for reviews). The operationalization of attachment varies across studies, with some researchers using interviews to evaluate attachment state-of-mind, while others re-lied on self-report measures, which encompassed assessments of both romantic attachment and general attachment orientations across diverse relationship contexts. Secure therapists tend to ex-hibit less negative countertransference behaviors, such as being critical or rejecting (Ligiéro & Gelso, 2002), are more attentive to the clients’ underlying needs, and provide relational experiences hat challenge pre-existing schemas or models of the world (Ben-nett, 2008; Dozier et al., 1994; Tyrrell et al., 1999), and tend to cope better with clients who report higher levels of distress during therapy (Mikulincer et al., 2013). A longitudinal study conducted by Sauer et al. (2003) has shown that anxious-attached psy-chotherapists developed stronger working alliances at the begin-ning of the therapy compared to less anxious-attached ones, but this pattern seems to be reverted during the therapeutic process.

If therapists who exhibit secure attachment status have been considered more responsive to their clients than their more in-secure counterparts, the underlying mechanism of this process is still largely unknown (Carvalho & Matos, 2021; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007; Talia et al., 2017; Talia et al., 2020). Consider-ing the potential effects that attachment might carry for psy-chotherapy, shedding light on the underlying processes that lead therapists to develop helpful relationships and work as secure base figures for their clients could be particularly valuable for training and supervision (Farber & Metzger, 2009; Pistole, 1999; Talia et al., 2019).

Therapists as secure base figures

The bridge between early relational experiences and beliefs and expectations in late relationships has been paved by central concepts in attachment theory, such as internal working models of self, others, and the social world (Thompson, 2008). However, it is in the secure base concept that Bowlby saw the greatest po-tential of attachment theory for counseling and psychotherapy. In fact, for Bowlby (1977, 1988), the therapist’s role as a secure base could provide the emotional conditions from which the clients could explore themselves and the world and begin to revise their representational models of themselves and others. Later, research has shown that client representations of the therapist as a secure base figure seem to be a critical predictor of positive outcomes (Goodwin et al., 2003; Levy et al., 2011; Mallinckrodt et al., 2017). Considering that psychotherapist-client relationships share several features of attachment relationships (Bowlby, 1988; Dozier et al., 1994; Levy & Johnson, 2019; Slade & Holmes, 2019) can offer meaningful contributions to understanding re-sponsiveness in psychotherapy, such as empathic concern or per-sonal distress (Fabi et al., 2019). Therapists’ representations of attachment can therefore inform secure base responses (Romano et al., 2008; Slade & Holmes, 2019; Talia et al., 2020), their men-talizing abilities, and nurturing and responsive behaviors in ther-apy (Talia et al., 2020).

Studies on the mentalizing stance of the therapist found that attachment affects the therapist’s mental states, which in turn are involved in alliance variations between dyads and over time (Bar-reto & Matos, 2022). However, there is still a lack of research op-erationalizing the therapeutic relationship from an attachment perspective (Steel et al., 2018). A critical dimension of secure base behavior was more recently explored by Talia et al. (2020). The authors found that psychotherapy’s distinctive patterns of attune-ment towards the patient were highly associated with the thera-pist’s attachment classification as assessed by a transcript-based instrument on therapeutic sessions (Talia et al., 2020). One inter-esting finding concerns the implications of therapists’ secure at-tachment status on their ability to be open-minded and deal with ambiguity and doubt (Talia et al., 2020).

In another study, Carvalho and Matos (2021) also presented the development of a self-report questionnaire for psychothera-pists’ ratings of secure base behaviors in psychotherapy in four distinctive dimensions [Secure Base Questionnaire (SBQ) – sensitivity, encouragement of exploration, compulsive caregiving, and avoidance of uncertainty]. They found that the therapist’s self-reported romantic attachment was associated with distinct repre-sentations of care and the client’s needs in therapy. Insecure dimensions of attachment, such as dependence and ambivalence, were associated with more insecure patterns of secure base pro-vision, namely with a higher prevalence of compulsive caregiving behaviors and more difficulties regarding tolerance for uncertainty in the psychotherapeutic process (Carvalho & Matos, 2021). Both of these studies provide additional proof of the therapist’s attach-ment role when facing more self-challenging experiences in psy-chotherapy. Emotional arousal could be triggered by emotionally distressing moments in psychotherapy, namely the ones that seem to question the psychotherapist’s competence in managing bound-aries, ruptures, and dealing with uncertainty (Barreto et al., 2020; Carvalho & Matos, 202; Mikulincer et al., 2013; Miller-Bottome et al., 2018; Rubino et al., 2000; Schauenburg et al., 2010).

The role of emotion regulation

Emotion regulation is a core construct in attachment theory (Bowlby, 1977; Cassidy, 1994; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2019) and refers to the processes that influence how and when emotions are experienced and expressed (Gross, 1988). More insecurely at-tached individuals face additional challenges when accessing emotions or experiencing heightened negative affect (Cassidy, 1994; Parada-Fernández et al., 2021; Shaver & Mikulincer, 2014). More specifically, avoidant people tend to block emotions that could threaten or promote vulnerability states, even if this implies dismissing their own emotional reactions. Differently, anxiously attached individuals’ hyperactivating strategies tend to exaggerate the appraisal process, heightening the threatful features of events and developing hypervigilant and pessimistic evaluations of their self-ability to manage distress (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2013, 2019).

In clinical settings, experiencing complex, diversifying, and chaotic feelings is inherent to the psychotherapist’s life (Najavits, 2000). From an attachment theoretical perspective, working with clients’ emotions can elicit discomfort in insecurely attached ther-apists, enhance withdrawal in more avoidant therapists, and in-crease over-involvement in anxiously attached ones (Degnan et al., 2016). Research found that a therapist’s attachment status could indirectly affect relational therapeutic dimensions and out-comes. Cologon et al. (2017) found that although attachment did not predict a therapist’s effectiveness, there was an important in-teraction effect with the psychotherapist’s reflective function.

Attachment security seems to compensate for low levels of reflective functioning, while high levels of reflective functioning seem to compensate for a more insecure attachment (Colognon et al., 2017). A similar finding was found with attachment and emo-tion regulation processes on relational dimensions, namely the working alliance (Ruiz-Aranda et al., 2021). Although there was no significant predicted effect of attachment on the therapeutic al-liance, moderation analysis yielded an interaction effect between attachment and emotion regulation (Ruiz-Aranda et al., 2021). In this study, a secure attachment did not necessarily predict better alliances since this relation depended on the therapists’ ability to understand and manage their emotions (Ruiz-Aranda et al., 2021). For example, when more insecure attached therapists were able to attend to and regulate their emotions, the therapeutic relation-ship was not affected by the quality of the therapist’s attachment (Ruiz-Aranda et al., 2021). It is also possible that when therapists feel emotionally overwhelmed, attachment security could hinder emotional triggers elicited by the moment. For instance, Fuerteset al. (2019) found that therapists with higher levels of anxiety and avoidance in attachment dimensions reported more difficulties in developing a genuine relationship with their clients. In this sense, emotion regulation processes can be considered necessary linking mechanisms to understand the complex dynamics between attachment and the provision of a secure base in the psychother-apeutic encounter.

Current study

Considering the above-mentioned literature, the main aim of this study is to analyze how current attachment dimensions are associated with how therapists represent themselves as secure base figures for their clients. We will also analyze the potential medi-ating role of emotion regulation in the previous association.

It is expected that secure attachment dimensions will be pos-itively related to dimensions more closely associated with a more secure script of the “secure base” (i.e., with SBQ’s dimensions of sensitivity and encouragement of exploration).

Similarly, it is expected that insecure attachment dimensions will be associated with over-involvement and difficulties in deal-ing with uncertainty during therapy (i.e., with SBQ’s dimensions of compulsive caregiving and avoidance of uncertainty) (Car-valho & Matos, 2016; Romano et al., 2008; Slade & Holmes, 2019; Talia et al., 2020). Emotional regulation processes are also expected to mediate therapists’ attachment representations and secure base provision in therapy (Degnan et al., 2016; Fuertes et al., 2019; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2014, 2019; Ruiz-Aranda et al., 2021).

Methods

Procedure

The sample was recruited over a period of 6 months, both on-line and offline. First, all societies and schools of psychotherapy in Portugal were reached to present the study and ask for collab-oration on the data collection process. Questionnaires were ad-ministered in loco or sent with a pre-stamped envelope. From a total of 512 questionnaires delivered, 230 were sent back (a return rate of 44.92%). Second, a snowball sampling technique was also used. A link to the online questionnaire (allocated by the faculty web services, in compliance with the legal and ethical require-ments, including the full anonymity of the respondents), was sent to a pool of psychologists and psychiatrists from all over the coun-try available online (universities, health services, private prac-tices), asking for their participation as well as to forward the email to other colleagues. A total of 154 questionnaires were collected through this method. The final sample included 40.1% of ques-tionnaires in web-based format and 59.9% in paper-and-pencil format. Participation was voluntary and anonymous, and no fi-nancial compensation was involved. The study followed all ethical requirements and was vetted and approved by the Scientific Com-mittee of the Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences of the University of Porto.

Participants

Other than being a psychotherapist, no inclusion/exclusion criteria were applied, including age, years of training, profession/position held, or experience. The sample used in the current study has been used in a previous study for addressing the psychometric properties of a self-report scale (Carvalho & Matos, 2021). The sample consisted of 384 psychotherapists, 73 males, and 311 females, ranging from 22 to 68 years old [mean (M)=33.3; standard deviation (SD)=8.05]. Years of experience ranged from 6 months to 40 years (M=7.8; SD=6.22). Regarding professional background, 358 were psychologists, 11 were psy-chiatrists, 8 were physicians with psychotherapeutic training, and 7 considered themselves psychotherapists but did not specify their professional background. The strong imbalance between males and females is not completely surprising, given that the majority of the sample is composed of psychologists, who are overwhelm-ingly women (in the Portuguese context). However, the imbalance between professional backgrounds, namely between psychologists and psychiatrists, is more difficult to explain, given the absence, to the best of our knowledge, of an authoritative source regarding the characterization of Portuguese psychotherapists.

Therapists were not forced to self-identify with one single theoretical orientation; rather, they were asked to rate (on a 1-5 Likert scale) the degree to which they identified with several theoretical orientations. For the descriptive analysis, we consider participants who responded 4 or 5 on the scale (i.e., “I identify myself with this orientation” and “I completely identify myself with this orientation”, respectively) as having a salient theoret-ical identification with that orientation. Consequently, percent-ages do add up to more than 100%. Most psychotherapists reported being influenced by two or more orientations (84.9%). The percentages for the most salient theoretical orientation re-ported were as follows: 48.9% of the therapists rated 4 or 5 for cognitive-behavioral, 50% for constructivist, 48.2% for human-istic/existential, 38.8% for integrative, 29.7% for psychoana-lytic/psychodynamic, and 64.3% for systemic. The implications of theoretical orientations on SBQ dimensions could be con-sulted elsewhere (Carvalho & Matos, 2021).

Clinical practices were mainly developed with adults (68.2%), followed by adolescents/young adults (44.8%), children (35.7%), and seniors (6%). Again, categories were not mutually exclusive.

Measures

Demographics and professional

Participants completed a demographic questionnaire ad-dressing age, gender, and professional dimensions, including questions on professional background, years of experience, and theoretical orientations. For assessing theoretical orientations, therapists were asked to indicate on a 5-point Likert-type scale to what extent they identified themselves with each of the fol-lowing six (not mutually exclusive) categories: cognitive-behav-ioral, constructivist, humanistic/existential, integrative, psychoanalytic, systemic, and other.

Attachment

For assessing the therapists’ attachment, the brief version of the Romantic Attachment Questionnaire (RAQ) (Matos & Costa, 2001), regarding the current or longest-lasting romantic relationship, was used. RAQ is a self-report measure designed to evaluate adult representations of romantic attachment. Prin-cipal component analysis and confirmatory factor analysis, using Portuguese independent samples (Ávila et al., 2012; Vieira et al., 2012), evidenced a reliable 4-factor structure. The brief ver-sion is composed of a total of 25 items, divided into four dimen-sions: i) trust [5 items, e.g, “I know that I can count on my partner whenever I need him/her” (α=.81)]; ii) ambivalence [8 items, e.g., “Sometimes I think that he/she is critical in my life, other times I don’t” (α=.78)]; iii) dependence [6 items, e.g., “When I cannot be with my partner, I feel abandoned” (α=.73)]; iv) avoidance [6 items, e.g., “When I have a problem, I prefer being alone instead of being with my partner” (α=.72)]. Ex-pected associations between RAQ dimensions and other con-structs, including subscale scores of the Experiences in Close Relationships Scale, were found to provide evidence for its con-struct validity (for a review, see Ávila et al., 2012).

Importantly, the RAQ was initially validated in Portuguese samples (Matos & Costa, 2001) and has since been used in several studies, particularly within the Portuguese context (including in the current study). Given its consistent record regarding psycho-metric properties, the RAQ is arguably the most suitable measure for assessing attachment in a sample of Portuguese therapists.

Emotion regulation 

To assess the psychotherapist’s ability to attend to, discrimi-nate against, and regulate emotions, the Portuguese version (Cabral et al., 2021) of the Trait-Meta Mood Scale (TMMS) (Sa-lovey et al., 1995) was used. The original (30-item) 3-factor so-lution proposed by Salovey et al. (1995) demonstrated evidence of convergent and discriminant validity, as well as internal con-sistency (Cronbach’s α ranging from .82 to .88.). A subsequent study (Palmer et al., 2003) replicated the 3-factor structure through both exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis and re-ported good internal consistency indicators (Cronbach’s α of .87 for clarity, .84 for attention, and .71 for repair). In the current study, we were able to achieve satisfactory values in confirmatory factor analysis, but only after adjustments: χ2/degrees of freedom (df)=4.00, comparative fit index (CFI)=.93, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA)=.080 [confidence interval (CI)=.060-.107].

Adjustments included the deletion of 8 items to improve the subscales’ internal consistency or because they presented double loadings. Consequently, the version of the scale used in this study is composed of 22 items aggregated in the three original dimen-sions: clarity (9 items, e.g., “I am rarely confused about how I feel”, α=.82); attention (8 items, e.g., “I do not pay much attention to my feelings”, α=.78) and repair (5 items, e.g., “I try to think good thoughts no matter how badly I feel”, α=.77).

Secure base

The SBQ (Carvalho & Matos, 2021) is a 17-item self-report questionnaire developed for assessing therapists’ representations as secure base figures for their clients. The questionnaire’s content validity was assured through input and revision by experienced senior researchers, and it included the establishment of the instru-ments’ facial validity with the participation of ten psychothera-pists. The final structure of the scale was achieved through principal component analysis (with varimax rotation), which re-sulted in a reliable and theoretically grounded 4-factor structure, explaining more than 51% of the variance: i) sensitivity [5 items, e.g., “I feel I am able to help my clients feel hope and security”, α=.69, mean inter-item correlation (MIC)=.31]; ii) compulsive caregiving (5 items, e.g., “I often feel overwhelmed by my client’s problems and difficulties”, α=.68, MIC=.30); iii) avoidance of un-certainty (4 items, e.g., “I feel uncomfortable when I face uncer-tainty in the therapeutic process”, α=.69, MIC=.36); iv) encouragement of exploration (3 items, e.g., “I usually encourage the client to reflect on his/her relational patterns”, α=.65, MIC=.39). Participants respond to a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Given the reduced num-ber of items per subscale and the relation between the number of items and Cronbach’s α, both Cronbach’s α (along with the num-ber of items in each subscale) and the MIC should be used to as-sess internal consistency (Field, 2018; Lance et al., 2006). As Cronbach’s α scores ranged from .65 to .69 and the lower MIC was .30, we conclude that all factors can be considered to present a good level of homogeneity.

Results

Preliminary and descriptive analysis

All scales used went through a confirmatory factor analysis process. When necessary, adjustments to the scales were made. Priority was given to the (re)validation of the original factor struc-ture achieved for the three used measures, as detailed in the mea-sure’s subsection.

The preliminary analysis included checking the percentage of missing values for each variable as well as the extent to which values of kurtosis and skewness were within reasonable limits. The percentage of missing data for each variable was considerably low, ranging from 0% to a maximum of 1.8%. As Newman (2014) recommended, maximum likelihood estimation (with robust stan-dard errors) was used. All but one variable were within the com-mon threshold of skewness below 3 and kurtosis above 10 (Kline, 2016; Weston & Gore, 2006).

Specifically, one item (TMMS_3_inv) presented a marginally higher value regarding skewness (-3.1) but a kurtosis value of 13.7. Given this item’s importance to the maintenance of the orig-inal factor structure, we opted to conduct a maximum likelihood estimation with robust (Huber-White) standard errors and the scaled chi-square statistic, therefore adjusting for the impact of non-normality on the results (Satorra & Bentler, 1988). Structural equation modeling (SEM) analysis, including confirmatory factor analyses and bootstrapping procedures, was conducted with R’s Lavaan package. The mediating bias-corrected bootstrap CI were generated with 1000 bootstrapping resamples and a 95% CI to test significance. If the 95% CI for the average estimates of these 1000 indirect effects did not include 0, using the method of Shrout and Bolger (2002) we concluded that the indirect effect is statis-tically significant at the .05 level.

M, SD, and 0-order correlations for all the 11 latent variables used in the SEM models are shown in Table 1.

Analytical plan

As a first step, we run a full model estimating all parameters between the latent variables (referred to below as the Initial Modeland presented in Figure 1), allowing for the identification of which independent variables (either directly or through mediation) car-ried any explanatory power over the dependent variables. Sec-ondly, after carefully analyzing these relations, variables that did not directly or through mediation present statistically significant relations with the dependent variables were removed. This re-sulted in the final model presented in Figure 2. For readability ease, neither Figure 1 nor 2 include the measurement model. More information regarding each measure is presented above under the Measures section. Nevertheless, it is important to make clear that no parameters were estimated directly between items. In other words, no item errors (or unique variances) were correlated.

Initial model

The adequacy of the models’ fit was based on the selection of the following indexes (DiStefano & Hesse, 2005; Hu & Bentler, 1999; Kline, 2016) and cut-off values: the ratio between (the scaled) χ2 statistic and degrees of freedom, with values below 3 considered acceptable and good below 2 (Schermelleh-Engel, et al., 2003); CFI, with values above .90 considered acceptable and good above .95 (Lance, et al., 2006; Schermelleh-Engel, et al., 2003); Tucker Lewis index (TLI), with values above .90 consid-ered acceptable and good above .95 (Hu & Bentler, 1999); RMSEA, with values below .08 considered acceptable and below .05 good (Hu & Bentler, 1999); and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), with values below .08 to be considered accept-able (Hu & Bentler, 1999).

 

Figure 1. Regression coefficients for mediation model testing the indirect/direct effects of attachment (trust, ambivalence, depend-ence and avoidance) on secure base behaviors toward clients (sen-sitivity, compulsive caregiving, avoidance of uncertainty and encouragement of exploration) through emotion regulation processes (clarity, attention and repair). Values are in standardized units. Grey lines represent non-significant paths. Solid lines depict direct effects. Dotted lines depict indirect effects. C. caregiving, compulsive caregiving; A. uncertainty, avoidance of uncertainty; E. exploration, encouragement of exploration.

The (exploratory) initial model depicted in Figure 1 pre-sented overall satisfactory adjustment indices. Specifically, the chi-square test statistic was 1381 for 890 df (χ2/df=1.55); CFI=.89; TLI=.88; RMSEA=.040 (.036-.044); and SRMR=.057. As depicted in Figure 1, several regression paths were deemed non-significant (grey lines).

As shown in Figure 1, RAQ’s avoidance does not directly or indirectly yield explanatory power over any outcome variables and was therefore removed from the final model. Contrarily, both RAQ’s dimensions of ambivalence and dependence presented a statistically significant direct path with SBQ’s outcomes and are therefore maintained in the final model. Furthermore, although RAQ’s trust factor was not directly associated with any of the out-comes, it showed statistically significant relations with SBQ’s sen-sitivity through the TMMS’s dimension of clarity. For this reason, both RAQ’s trust and the mediator’s clarity were maintained in the final model.

As TMMS’s dimension repair did not yield statistically sig-nificant associations with both RAQ’s and SBQ’s dimensions, they were not included in the final model. Hence, the final model to be tested is composed of three RAQ’s dimensions (trust, am-bivalence, and dependence), clarity as a mediator, and the four SBQ outcomes (sensitivity, compulsive caregiving, avoidance of uncertainty, and encouragement of exploration).

Final model

The resulting final model, with the above-specified variables, is depicted in Figure 2. Adjustment indexes confirm an overall good fit of the model to the data, according to the above-specified thresholds. Specifically: the chi-square (scaled robust) statistic was 757 for 473 df (χ2/df=1.60); CFI=.92; TLI=.91; RMSEA=.041 (.036-.047); and SRMR=.058. As observable in Figure 2, RAQ’s trust dimension has been shown to be directly and positively associated with SBQ’s com-pulsive caregiving. Ambivalence has yielded positive relations with both SBQ’s compulsive caregiving and avoidance of uncer-tainty, but not with exploration or sensitivity. Lastly, RAQ’s de-pendence has proved to relate to both compulsive caregiving and avoidance of uncertainty (but not with encouragement of explo-ration or sensitivity).

Figure 2. Regression coefficients for mediation model testing the indirect/direct effects of attachment (trust, ambivalence, de-pendence) on secure base behaviors toward clients (sensitivity, compulsive caregiving, avoidance of uncertainty and encourage-ment of exploration) through emotion regulation processes (clar-ity). Values are in standardized units. Grey lines represent non-significant paths. Solid lines depict direct effects. Dotted lines depict indirect effects. C. caregiving, compulsive caregiv-ing; A. uncertainty, avoidance of uncertainty; E. exploration, en-couragement of exploration.

Mediation analysis Clarity has been shown to mediate the relation between RAQ’s trust dimension and three of the four outcome variables, with none of the three 95% bootstrap CI including 0. In more de-tail, clarity has proven to fully mediate the relation between trust, on the one hand, and sensitivity and encouragement of explo-ration, on the other. Additionally, clarity partly mediates the as-sociation between RAQ’s trust and SBQ’s compulsive caregiving. Lastly, the indirect effects between trust and avoidance of uncer-tainty through clarity did not reach statistical significance. Indirect effects estimates are presented in Table 2.

Discussion

The implications of psychotherapists’ personal characteristics, namely attachment dimensions, on psychotherapy have been con-sistently addressed by research (Bennett, 2008; Dozier et al., 1994; Fuertes et al., 2019; Lingiardi et al., 2018; Ryan et al., 2023; Tyrrell et al., 1999). Nevertheless, scarce knowledge has been produced regarding the underlying mechanisms of these processes (Carvalho & Matos, 2021; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007; Talia et al., 2017; Talia et al., 2020). By framing therapeutic relationships through the complementarity between attachment and caregiving systems, this study offers important contributions for gaining a deeper knowledge of the processes implicated in the therapeutic encounter, namely the ones deeply rooted in relational dimensions such as attachment and emotion regulation.

One contribution relates to the association between attach-ment dimensions and the provision of a secure base in clinical set-tings. This result brings further evidence to research that addresses the relational dimensions of therapy and the crucial role that per-sonal characteristics play in the way therapists represent them-selves as secure base figures in psychotherapy (Daniel, 2006; Degnan et al., 2016; Steel et al., 2018). Additionally, this result confirms previous studies that show psychotherapy is significantly influenced by the complex dynamics underlying therapists’ his-tory of receiving care (Slade & Holmes, 2019) and therapists’ pre-dispositions to form specific relationships with others (Steel et al., 2018). In this context, SBQ (Carvalho & Matos, 2021) offers an essential tool for capturing different dimensions associated with a central and subtle concept of attachment relationships in adult-hood – the secure base one (Crowell et al., 2002; Waters & Cum-mings, 2000).

The second contribution relies on the possibility offered by this study to develop a closer look at the processes underlying the link between attachment and relational dimensions in the psy-chotherapeutic context. Distinct interactions were found between attachment insecurity/security dimensions and the psychothera-pists’ representation as a secure base figure for clients. Direct ef-fects have shown that ambivalence and dependence attachment dimensions are primarily and positively associated with psy-chotherapists’ compulsive caregiving and avoidance of uncer-tainty (Carvalho & Matos, 2021). Both compulsive caregiving and the avoidance of uncertainty could work as important emo-tional triggers for psychotherapists with insecure internal working models. Previous research has found that these components of caregiving in adult relationships could be primarily found in in-dividuals who are more prone to experiencing negative models of themselves, namely anxious and fearful attachment patterns (Clark etal., 2020; Kunce & Shaver, 1994; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2013; Shaver et al., 2005).

In fact, fusional and preoccupied care behaviors can also be characteristics of close and intimate relationships (Bowlby, 1977). This is particularly relevant in the therapeutic process since most clients seeking professional help experience emotionally vulner-able states and are eager for genuine and interested care (Farber & Metzger, 2009). Sauer et al. (2003) provided additional infor-mation regarding the increased ability of anxious attached psy-chotherapists to develop stronger working alliances at the beginning of the therapy; nevertheless, from a processual view, this bond could be more fragile and less resistant to therapeutic tensions or challenges. Compulsive care could be particularly detrimental during the intermediate phase of the therapy when therapists and clients have a lower chance of accordance (Kanni-nen et al., 2000). As in other adult relational dynamics, exerting a more controlled and fusional relationship may respond to emo-tionally perceived threats of abandonment or being inadequate (Bowlby, 1977; Kunce & Shaver, 1994). 

In the same way, avoidance of uncertainty could be significant for engaging in reassurance-seeking processes and dealing with uncertainty processes in therapy (Clark et al., 2020; Shaver et al., 2005). Less positive models of self-constraint, a more open atti-tude and flexibility toward new information, and ambivalence can ultimately prevent the embracement of uncertainty in the thera-peutic process (Mikulincer, 1997; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2013). These results align with previous research that shows that therapist attachment threats could be activated in more stressful moments in therapy or when relating to more challenging clients (Sauer, 2003; Schauenburg et al., 2010).

Another significant result closely connected to this last con-tribution regards the absence of emotion regulation as a mediator between ambivalence and dependence and the secure base dimen-sions in clinical settings. It is possible that hyperactivation attach-ment strategies (primarily used in individuals with higher scores on dependence and ambivalence attachment dimensions) or higher vulnerability to activation of own attachment-related wor-ries during psychotherapy (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2013, 2019) could undermine the therapist’s ability to consciously acknowl-edge these processes in the therapeutic relationship. Paralleling other regulation processes in psychotherapy, such as mentaliza-tion, hyperactivation may hinder the capacity to integrate uncer-tainty and develop a curiosity attitude towards the therapeutic process. These processes could ultimately interfere with the psy-chotherapist’s ability to be responsive to the client’s underlying attachment needs (Bowlby, 1988) or even heighten the patient’s defenses in therapy (Slade & Holmes, 2019) since the therapist’s own attachment fears overlap with responsive caregiving centered on the client’s attachment needs.

A different result has been shown by the secure attachment dimension represented by trust. Two distinct associations were found between trust and compulsive caregiving through a me-diated and a direct path. The mediating effect of clarity had two major implications for the association between psychotherapist attachment and secure base representations. First, we observed a negative association between these two dimensions through clarity. Second, there was a positive association between trust and representations of compulsive care provision in therapy. Note that the correlation analysis shows a negative association between trust and compulsive caregiving, indicating that even when inspecting the direct effects, emotion regulation processes interfere with this direct association. These results stress the complex nature of the dynamics involved in providing secure base processes in psychotherapy, requiring an integrated analysis of this effect. In this sense, there are facets of compulsive care-giving that could be shared by secure and some insecure attach-ment dimensions. The distinctive processes of the secure and insecure dimensions concern the role of the emotional regulation process. The ability to acknowledge one’s own emotions affects experiences and states involved in the emotion regulation process of clarity, which could, as in other relational dimensions of the therapeutic encounter, facilitate empathic processes and re-centered the psychotherapist’s focus on the client’s attach-ment needs (Ruiz-Aranda et al., 2021; Ryan et al., 2023). This mechanism could be fundamental in situations where the thera-pist is most reflexive or more uncertain about the therapeutic re-lationship and process. An interesting finding of this study is that higher levels of trust do not seem to prevent therapist care-giving representations of compulsive caregiving behaviors in psychotherapy. Nevertheless, this association is challenged by emotional self-awareness processes induced by clarity. Emotion regulation processes allow therapists to attend and gain a deeper understanding of their own emotions and feelings in this process. Clarity was also an essential mediator between trust and the en-couragement of exploration in clinical settings. Note that al-though the magnitudes of effects were smaller when addressing indirect paths (Table 2), this study offers important ground by acknowledging the role of emotion regulation as one of the mechanisms that could interfere with relational processes in psy-chotherapy. This finding lends support to earlier research that addresses the critical role of the therapists’ emotion regulation process on their ability to develop responsive therapeutic rela-tionships (Ruiz-Aranda et al., 2021). 

Additionally, this study offers essential ground for disentan-gling challenges that emerge from psychotherapeutic processes, such as questioning the proximity and investment in therapy and dealing with the uncertainty/erratic nature of the therapeutic process and the mechanisms underlying responses in therapists that exhibited distinct levels of secure and insecure attachment di-mensions. The challenge is to understand if these mechanisms, which may function as potentially protective mediator factors, can be (and in what ways) improved in therapeutic training and su-pervision. Given the “unconscious predispositions of therapists to form certain styles of relationships with themselves and others” (Steel et al., 2018, p. 34), it would also be interesting to understand how clients’ distinct attachment dimensions elicit distinct re-sponses as secure base figures, as well as the role of emotion reg-ulation processes in this association.

Several limitations and constraints should be acknowledged in this study. The first one concerns the small ratio between sam-ple size and the number of parameters to be estimated, which may warrant some caution in the interpretation of the results (Bentler & Chou, 1987; Kline, 2016; Mueller, 1997). Nevertheless, the rel-atively large sample size of therapists (n=384), which is signifi-cantly above the commonly suggested 200 thresholds (Jackson, 2003), the reliability of the indicators used, and the adequate num-ber of indicators per latent variable important criteria mentioned by different authors (Bentler & Chou, 1987; Jackson, 2001, 2003; Kline, 2015; Mueller, 1997), give robustness to our findings.

The second one concerns the use of exclusive psychothera-pist self-report assessments for evaluating relational dynamics in psychotherapy. Research has stressed the differentiating role that client attachment could elicit in therapist responses, namely in the most insecure ones (for a review, see Daniel, 2006; Ro-mano et al., 2008). As previously stated, future studies should acknowledge these interactional processes that collect data on both clients’ and therapists’ personal and relational characteris-tics. Considering potential limitations to dyadic data collection in real settings, using performance-based tasks and observational studies could be an important alternative for addressing therapists’ contributions, such as attachment, to therapeutic processes and outcomes (Anderson et al., 2020) and to control potential self-appraisal bias. Secondly, considering the bidirectional in-fluences between psychotherapeutic processes, outcomes, and psychotherapist attachment (Degnan et al., 2016), future studies should include outcome variables and dyadic and longitudinal designs. In fact, the cross-sectional nature of the current study represents an explicit limitation, namely when addressing such dynamic dimensions of the therapeutic encounter. Multiformat and repeated measures design could help us gain a more com-prehensive insight into the stability of secure base constructs throughout distinctive therapeutic dyads and across therapeutic moments.

Contributions to psychotherapeutic training, supervision and practices

It appears that the change process among this small and unique sample of therapists closely parallels how clients change during therapy: Within a supportive interpersonal context, the person becomes aware of things in one ́s life that are remnants of the past and do not necessarily work in the current situation (…) This is how our clients change. As Sullivan once suggested, people are more human than otherwise (Goldfried, 2001, p. 326)

While there is a consistent body of research addressing the impactful nature of therapists’ relational characteristics in psy-chotherapeutic processes and outcomes, there are still scarcely any specific guidelines for helping psychotherapists gain a deeper understanding of their own relational stories. Attachment history could have complex, unconscious, and dynamic impli-cations for therapeutic interactions that could inspire or inhibit change. Nevertheless, offering space-time opportunities for im-proving self-knowledge of emotional triggers and working on emotion regulation processes could enhance therapists’ under-standing of both their own and client’s mental states (Ryan et al., 2023).

Supervision and intervision practices should offer secure base contexts for improving therapists’ emotional awareness by exploring distinct emotional experiences elicited by clients’ dis-tinct relational patterns, distressful events, and working alliance challenges. In this sense, SBQ could be a useful tool for moni-toring processes and ultimately enhancing mentalizing abilities as an important mechanism for compensating insecure attach-ment patterns of oneself and others (Cologon et al., 2017). Also, considering the close link between attachment and secure base behaviors in psychotherapy, psychotherapists’ training could benefit from intervention strategies used in attachment-based in-terventions for improving sensitivity and attunement.

Psychotherapists’ attachment-related issues should be further recognized and explored using distinct strategies that go from video feedback, discussion of research on therapist and client ef-fects on therapeutic sessions and offering individual psychological support for helping therapists improve mentalization abilities, in-tegrating trauma, or the most difficult emotional experiences and memories (Dozier et al., 2018; Talia et al., 2019; Woodhouse et al., 2018). The supervising/training relationship could offer an op-timal context for framing the therapeutic act while considering the relational attachment framework, promoting improvements in sensitive and attunement abilities but also preventing conditions for feeling unhelpful and, ultimately, burnout (Hiebler-Ragger et al., 2021; Talia et al., 2019).

Conclusions

In summary, this study supports the role of emotional regula-tion processes, namely concerning the ability to attend, discrimi-nate, and acknowledge emotions, as robust processes for grasping therapeutic outcomes (Najavits, 2000). Humanizing the therapeu-tic encounter also implies recognizing that psychotherapists’ ac-tions are, as in their clients, inspired by their history of love and affection, their emotional comprehension of human nature, and what thrives motivation and behaviors. If there is a profound eth-ical compromise in becoming better professionals, providing bet-ter responses, and helping people thrive through “dark waters”, acknowledging and considering these profound human dimen-sions of care could also contribute to improving the psychothera-pists’ sense of self-worth, competence, and security, which are necessary conditions for embracing uncertainty and complexity in psychotherapy (Stricker, 2002).

Contributions: all the authors made a substantial intellectual con-tribution, read and approved the final version of the manuscript, and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Conflict of interest: the authors declare no potential conflict of in-terest.

Ethics approval and consent to participate: the study followed all ethical requirements and was vetted and approved by the Scientific Committee of the Faculty of Psychology and Education Sciences of the University of Porto.

Informed consent: not applicable. Funding: this work has been funded by Foundation for Science and Technology (FCT), namely through grants SFRH/BD/22333/2005 (Helena Carvalho); UIDB000502020 and CEECIND/00646/2018 (Gil Nata).

Anderson, T., Finkelstein, J. D., & Horvath, S. A. (2020) The fa-cilitative interpersonal skills method: difficult psychotherapy moments and appropriate therapist responsiveness. Counsel-ing Psychotherapy Research, 20, 463-469. doi: 10.1002/capr. 12302.

Ávila, M., Cabral, J., & Matos, P. M. (2012). Identity in university students: the role of parental and romantic attachment. Jour-nal of Adolescence, 35(1), 133-142. doi: 10.1016/j.adoles-cence.2011.05.002.

Barreto, J. F., & Matos, P. M. (2022). Attachment mismatches and alliance: through the pitfalls of mentalizing countertransfer-ence. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 39(3), 276-279. doi: 10.1037/pap0000410.

Barreto, J. F., Nata, G., & Matos, P. M. (2020). Elaboration of countertransference experience and the workings of the work-ing alliance. Psychotherapy, 57(2), 141-150. doi: 10.1037/ pst0000250.

Bennett, S. C. (2008). The interface of attachment, transference, and countertransference: implications for the clinical super-visory relationship. Smith College Studies in Social Work,78(2-3), 301-320. doi: 10.1080/00377310802114635.

Bentler, P. M., & Chou, C. P. (1987). Practical issues in structural modeling. Sociological Methods & Research, 16(1), 78-117. doi: 10.1177/0049124187016001004. Bowlby, J. (1977). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. II. Some principles of psychotherapy. The fiftieth Maudsleylecture. British Journal of Psychiatry, 130, 421-431. doi:10.1192/bjp.130.5.421.

Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: parent-child attachment and healthy human development. Basic Books. Cabral, J., Brandão, T., Lamela, D., & Matos, P. (2021). The psy-chometric properties of a Portuguese version of the Trait-Meta Mood Scale: an attachment framework. Journal of Psy-chopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 43, 162-173. doi: 10.1007/s10862-020-09817-4.

Carvalho, H. M., & Matos, P. M. (2016). Being and becoming a psychotherapist in Portugal: a dialogue between personal and professional experiences. In L. Jennings & T. M Skovholt (Eds.), Expertise in counseling and psychotherapy: master therapist studies from around the world (pp. 93-126). Oxford University Press.

Carvalho, H. M., & Matos, P. M. (2021). Psychotherapist as a se-cure base figure: validation of the Secure Base Questionnaire (SBQ). Professional Psychology: Research and Practice,52(4), 396-405. doi: 10.1037/pro0000366.

Cassidy, J. (1994). Emotion regulation: influences of attachment relationships. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2-3), 228-249. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.1994.tb01287.x.

Clark, G. I., Rock, A. J., Clark, L. H., & Murraylyon, K. (2020). Adult attachment, worry and reassurance seeking: investigat-ing the role of intolerance of uncertainty. Clinical Psycholo-gist, 24(3), 294-305. doi: 10.1111/cp.12218.

Cologon, J., Schweitzer, R. D., King, R., & Nolte, T. (2017). Ther-apist reflective functioning, therapist attachment style and therapist effectiveness. Administration and Policy in Mental Health and Mental Health Services Research, 44(5), 614-625. doi: 10.1007/s10488-017-0790-5.

Crowell, J. A., Treboux, D., & Waters, E. (2002). Stability of at-tachment representations: the transition to marriage. Devel-opmental Psychology, 38(4), 467-479. doi: 10.1037/0012- 1649.38.4.467.

Daniel, S. I. (2006). Adult attachment patterns and individual psy-chotherapy: a review. Clinical Psychology Review, 26(8), 968-984. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.02.001.

Degnan, A., Seymour-Hyde, A., Harris, A., & Berry, K. (2016). The role of therapist attachment in alliance and outcome: a systematic literature review. Clinical Psychology and Psy-chotherapy, 23(1), 47-65. doi: 10.1002/cpp.1937.

DiStefano, C., & Hess, B. (2005). Using confirmatory factor analysis for construct validation: an empirical review. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 23(3), 225-241. doi: 10.1177/073428290502300303.

Dozier, M., Bernard, K., & Roben, C. (2018). Attachment and biobehavioral Catch-up. In H. S. M. Steele (Ed.), Handbook of attachment-based interventions. The Guildford Press. Dozier, M., Davis, K., & Barnett, L. (1994). Clinicians as care-givers: role of attachment organization in treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62(4), 793-800. doi: 10.1037//0022-006X.62.4.793.

Fabi, S., Weber, L. A., & Leuthold, H. (2019). Empathic concern and personal distress depend on situational but not disposi-tional factors. PLoS One, 14(11), e0225102. doi: 10.1371/ journal.pone.0225102.

Farber, B. A., & Metzger, J. A. (2009). The therapist as secure base. In J. H. Obegi & E. Berant (Eds.), Attachment theory and research in clinical work with adults (pp. 46-70). The Guilford Press. Field, A. (2018). Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statis-tics (5th ed.). Sage Publications Ltd.

Fuertes, J. N., Moore, M., & Ganley, J. (2019). Therapists’ and clients’ ratings of real relationship, attachment, therapist self-disclosure, and treatment progress. Psychotherapy Research,29(5), 594-606. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2018.1425929.

Goldfried, M. R. (2001). How therapists change: personal andprofessional reflections. American Psychological Association. doi: 10.1037/10392-000.

Goodwin, I., Holmes, G., Cochrane, R., & Mason, O. (2003). The ability of adult mental health services to meet clients’ attach-ment needs: the development and implementation of the Serv-ice Attachment Questionnaire. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 76(2), 145-161. doi: 10.1348/147608303765951186.

Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 271-299. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271.

Heinonen, E., & Nissen-Lie, H. A. (2020). The professional and personal characteristics of effective psychotherapists: a sys-tematic review. Psychotherapy Research, 30(4), 417-432. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2019.1620366.

Hiebler-Ragger, M., Nausner, L., Blaha, A., Grimmer, K., Korlath, S., Mernyi, M., & Unterrainer, H. F. (2021). The supervisory relationship from an attachment perspective: connections to burnout and sense of coherence in health professionals. Clin-ical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 28(1), 124-136. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2494.

Holmes, J. (2011). Attachment in the consulting room: towards a theory of therapeutic change. European Journal of Psy-chotherapy & Counselling, 13(2), 97-114. doi: 10.1080/ 13642537.2011.570013.

Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1-55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118.

Jackson, D. L. (2003). Revisiting sample size and number of pa-rameter estimates: some support for the N: q hypothesis. Structural equation modeling,10(1), 128-141. doi: 10.1207/ S15328007SEM1001_6.

Jackson, D. L. (2001). Sample size and the number of parameter estimates in maximum likelihood confirmatory factor analy-sis: a Monte Carlo investigation. Structural Equation Model-ing, 8(2), 205-223. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM0802_3.

Kanninen, K., Salo, J., & Punamäki, R. L. (2000). Attachment patterns and working alliance in trauma therapy for victims of political violence. Psychotherapy Research, 10(4), 435-449. doi: 10.1093/ptr/10.4.435.

Kline, R. B. (2016). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (4th ed.). The Guilford Press. Kunce, L. J., & Shaver, P. R. (1994). An attachment-theoretical approach to caregiving in romantic relationships. In K. Bartholomew & D. Perlman (Eds.), Attachment processes in adulthood (pp. 205-237).

Jessica Kingsley Publishers. Lance, C. E., Butts, M. M., & Michels, L. C. (2006). The sources of four commonly reported cutoff criteria. What did they re-ally say? Organizational Research Methods, 9(2), 202-220. doi: 10.1177/1094428105284919.

Levy, K. N., Ellison, W. D., Scott, L. N., & Bernecker, S. L. (2011). Attachment style. Journal of Clinical Psychology,67(2), 193-203. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20756.

Levy, K. N., & Johnson, B. N. (2019). Attachment and psy-chotherapy: implications from empirical research. Canadian Psychology, 60(3), 178-193. doi: 10.1037/cap0000162.

Ligiéro, D. P., & Gelso, C. J. (2002). Countertransference, attach-ment, and the working alliance: the therapist’s contribution. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 39(1), 3-11. doi: 10.1037/0033-3204.39.1.3.

Lingiardi, V., Muzi, L., Tanzilli, A., & Carone, N. (2018). Do ther-apists’ subjective variables impact on psychodynamic psy-chotherapy outcomes? A systematic literature review. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 25(1), 85-101. doi: 10.1002/ cpp.2131.

Mallinckrodt, B., Anderson, M. Z., Choi, G., Levy, K. N., Petrowski, K., Sauer, E. M., Tishby, O., & Wiseman, H. (2017). Pseudosecure vs. individuated-secure client attach-ment to therapist: implications for therapy process and out-come. Psychotherapy Research, 27(6), 677-691. doi: 10.1080/ 10503307.2016.1152411.

Matos, P. M., & Costa, M. E. (2001). Questionário de Vinculação Amorosa [Romantic Attachment Questionnaire]. Unpublished manuscript. Faculty of Psychology and Sciences of Education University of Porto.

Mikulincer, M. (1997). Adult attachment style and information processing: Individual differences in curiosity and cognitive closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(5), 1217-1230. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.72.5.1217.

Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: structure, dynamics and change. Guilford Press.

Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2019). Attachment orientations and emotion regulation. Current Opinion in Psychology, 25, 6-10. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.02.006.

Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., & Berant, E. (2013). An attachment perspective on therapeutic processes and outcomes. Journal of Personality, 81(6), 606-616. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494. 2012.00806.x.

Miller-Bottome, M., Talia, A., Safran, J. D., & Muran, J. C. (2018). Resolving alliance ruptures from an attachment-in-formed perspective. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 35(2), 175-183. doi: 10.1037/pap0000152.

Mueller, R. O. (1997). Structural equation modeling: back to the basics. Structural Equation Modeling, 4(4), 353-369. doi: 10.1080/10705519709540081.

Najavits, L. M. (2000). Researching therapist emotions and coun-tertransference. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 7(3), 322-328. doi: 10.1016/S1077-7229(00)80090-5.

Newman, D. A. (2014). Missing data: five practical guidelines. Organizational Research Methods, 17(4), 372-411. doi: 10.1177/1094428114548590.

Palmer, B., Gignac, G., Bates, T., & Stough, C. (2003). Exam-ining the structure of the trait metamood scale. Australian Journal of Psychology, 55(3), 154-158. doi: 10.1080/ 0004953042000298612.

Parada-Fernández, P., Herrero-Fernández, D., Oliva-Macías, M., & Rohwer, H. (2021). Analysis of the mediating effect of mentalization on the relationship between attachment styles and emotion dysregulation. Scandinavian Journal of Psychol-ogy, 62(3), 312-320. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12717.

Pistole, M. C. (1999). Caregiving in attachment relationships: a perspective for counselors. Journal of Counseling & Devel-opment, 77(4), 437-446. doi: 10.1002/j.1556-6676.1999. tb02471.x.

Romano, V., Fitzpatrick, M., & Janzen, J. (2008). The secure-base hypothesis: global attachment, attachment to counselor, and session exploration in psychotherapy. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 55(4), 495-504. doi: 10.1037/a0013721.

Rubino, G., Barker, C., Roth, T., & Fearon, P. (2000). Therapist empathy and depth of interpretation in response to potential alliance ruptures: the role of therapist and patient attachment styles. Psychotherapy Research, 10(4), 408-420. doi: 10.1093/ ptr/10.4.408.

Ruiz-Aranda, D., Cardoso-Álvarez, S., & Fenollar-Cortés, J. (2021). Therapist attachment and the working alliance: The moderating effect of emotional regulation. Frontiers in Psy-chology, 12, 784010. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.784010.

Ryan, R., Berry, K., & Hartley, S. (2023). Therapist factors and their impact on therapeutic alliance in child and adolescent mental health: a systematic review. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 28(2), 195-211. doi: 10.1111/camh.12518.

Salovey, P., Mayer, J. D., Goldman, S., Turvey, C., & Palfai, T. (1995). Emotional attention, clarity and repair: exploring emotional intelligence using the Trait Meta-Mood scale. In J. W. Pennebaker (Ed.), Emotion, disclosure and health (pp. 125-154). American Psychological Association.

Sauer, E. M., Lopez, F. G., & Gormley, B. (2003). Respective con-tributions of therapist and client attachment orientations to the development of the early working alliance: a preliminary growth modeling study. Psychotherapy Research, 13(3), 371-382. doi: 10.1093/ptr/kpg027.

Schauenburg, H., Buchheim, A., Beckh, K., Nolte, T., Brenk-Franz, K., Leichsenring, F., Strack, M., & Dinger, U. (2010). The influence of psychodynamically oriented therapists’ at-tachment representations on outcome and alliance in inpatient psychotherapy. Psychotherapy Research, 20(2), 193-202. doi: 10.1080/10503300903204043.

Schermelleh-Engel, K., Moosbrugger, H., & Müller, H. (2003). Evaluating the fit of structural equation models: tests of sig-nificance and descriptive goodness-of-fit measures. Methods of Psychological Research Online, 8(2), 23-74. Shaver, P. R., Schachner, D. A., & Mikulincer, M. (2005). Attach-ment style, excessive reassurance seeking, relationship processes, and depression. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(3), 343-359. doi: 10.1177/0146167204271709.

Shaver, P. R., & Mikulincer, M. (2014). Adult attachment and emotion regulation. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 237-250). The Guilford Press. Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommenda-tions. Psychological Methods, 7(4), 422-445. doi: 10.1037/ 1082-989X.7.4.422.

Slade, A., & Holmes, J. (2019). Attachment and psychotherapy. Current Opinion in Psychology, 25, 152-156. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.06.008.

Steel, C., Macdonald, J. and Schroder, T. (2018). A systematic re-view of the effect of therapists’ internalized models of rela-tionships on the quality of the therapeutic relationship. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 74(1), 5-42. doi: 10.1002/ jclp.22484.

Stricker, G. (2002), What is a scientist-practitioner anyway? Jour-nal of Clinical Psychology, 58(10), 1277-1283. doi: 10.1002/j clp.10111.

Talia, A., Miller-Bottome, M., & Daniel, S. I. F. (2017). Assessing attachment in psychotherapy: validation of the patient attach-ment coding system (PACS). Clinical Psychology & Psy-chotherapy, 24(1), 149-161. doi: 10.1002/cpp.1990.

Talia, A., Taubner, S., & Miller-Bottome, M. (2019). Advances in research on attachment-related psychotherapy processes: seven teaching points for trainees and supervisors. Research in Psychotherapy: Psychopathology, Process and Outcome, 22(3), 405. doi: 10.4081/ripppo.2019.405.

Talia, A., Muzi, L., Lingiardi, V., & Taubner, S. (2020). How to be a secure base: therapists’ attachment representations and their link to attunement in psychotherapy. Attachment & Human Development, 22(2), 189-206. doi: 10.1080/ 14616734.2018.1534247.

Thompson, R. A. (2008). Attachment-related mental represen-tations: introduction to the special issue. Attachment & Human Development, 10(4), 347-358. doi: 10.1080/ 14616730802461334.

Tyrrell, C. L., Dozier, M., Teague, G. B., & Fallot, R. D. (1999). Effective treatment relationships for persons with serious psy-chiatric disorders: the importance of attachment states of mind. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67(5), 725-733. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.67.5.725.

Vieira, J., Ávila, M., & Matos, P. M. (2012). Attachment and parenting: the mediating role of work-family balance in Por-tuguese parents of pre-school children. Family Relations, 61(1), 31-50. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3729.2011 .00680.x.

Waters, E., & Cummings, E. M. (2000). A secure base from which to explore close relationships. Child Development, 71(1), 164-172. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00130.

Weston, R., & Gore, P. A. (2006). A brief guide to structural equa-tion modeling. Counseling Psychologist, 34(5), 719-751. doi: 10.1177/0011000006286345.

Woodhouse, S. S., Powell, B., Cooper, G., Hoffman, K., & Cas-sidy, J. (2018). The circle of security intervention: Design, re-search, and implementation. In H. Steele & M. Steele (Eds.), Handbook of attachment-based interventions (pp. 50-78). The Guilford Press.

 

www.wappc.org | mail@wappc.org

© World Association for the Development of Psychotherapy and Psychological Counseling